Blog Post 8 - Automimicry


In this week’s post I will introduce the concept of automimicry. Many prey species carry primary defences like warning colouration and secondary defences such as toxins and stings, which are harmful to predators, but cannot be detected by predators before the prey is captured (Svennungsen and Holen, 2007; Ruxton and Speed, 2006).  Predators that attack defended prey usually learn to avoid similar-looking prey by associating the prey's visual characteristics with its defence. Because the warning signal is typically structurally unrelated to the secondary defence, it is not necessarily an honest indicator of a prey's true defence level. Therefore, secondary defences are readily exploitable by undefended ‘cheats’ that carry only the deceptive warning signal (Svennungsen and Holen, 2007). Such exploitation is illustrated in the previously discussed concept of Batesian mimicry, where one or several undefended species benefit from mimicking the appearance of a defended model species. Another intriguing possibility, which is the focus of this blog, is that of automimicry, in which some fraction of the individuals in a defended species produces little or nothing of the defence, but nevertheless obtains some protection from predation because they are identical in appearance to their defended conspecifics (Speed et al. 2006).

In automimicry, the replication of patterns present in conspecific individuals at homologous sites or elsewhere in the body can take a few forms. It can occur through behaviour where an individual will mimic the gestures of another class of individuals, and it can also occur through morphology where colour patterns and threatening body parts are repeated and used for intimidation and defence (Guthrie and Petocz, 1970). Automimicry seems to follow the concept of one works well, and two work even better (Guthrie and Petocz, 1970). So basically, as long as an automimic prey species looks and acts like it has that secondary defence, even though it doesn’t, it is usually avoided by predators.

There are many examples of automimicry. Some include the gray hairstreak butterfly (Strymon melinus) which have a false head at the base of their wings deflecting attacks from the actual head. The foureye butterflyfish (Chaetodon casistratus) mimic their own eyes on their back to also deflect attacks from the vulnerable head. Also many males of various bee and wasp species, although defenceless, are protected from predators by their resemblance to females that are equipped with stingers.

The gray hairstreak butterfly (Strymon melinus) Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Automimicry#/media/File:Gray_Hairstreak_(One_more_time...)_(6222138633).jpg Retrieved on: 03/05/2019
 
The foureye butterflyfish (Chaetodon casistratus) Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Automimicry#/media/File:Chaetodon_capistratus2.jpg Retrieved on: 03/05/2019


References



Guthrie, R.D. and Petocz, R.G., 1970. Weapon automimicry among mammals. The American Naturalist, 104(940), pp.585-588.

Ruxton, G.D. and Speed, M.P., 2005. How can automimicry persist when predators can preferentially consume undefended mimics?. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1584), pp.373-378.

Speed, M.P., Ruxton, G.D. and Broom, M., 2006. Automimicry and the evolution of discrete prey defences. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 87(3), pp.393-402.

Svennungsen, T.O. and Holen, Ø.H., 2007. The evolutionary stability of automimicry. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1621), pp.2055-2063.

Comments

  1. Automimicry seems to be quite a curious thing. I assume it’s something to do with underlying genetic changes that result in the secondary defence (e.g. the toxin) not being expressed?

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog