Blog Post 8 - Automimicry
In this week’s post
I will introduce the concept of automimicry. Many prey species carry
primary defences like warning colouration and secondary defences such as toxins
and stings, which are harmful to predators, but cannot be detected by predators
before the prey is captured (Svennungsen and Holen, 2007; Ruxton and Speed,
2006). Predators that attack defended
prey usually learn to avoid similar-looking prey by associating the prey's
visual characteristics with its defence. Because the warning signal is
typically structurally unrelated to the secondary defence, it is not
necessarily an honest indicator of a prey's true defence level. Therefore,
secondary defences are readily exploitable by undefended ‘cheats’ that carry
only the deceptive warning signal (Svennungsen and Holen, 2007). Such
exploitation is illustrated in the previously discussed concept of Batesian
mimicry, where one or several undefended species benefit from mimicking the
appearance of a defended model species. Another intriguing possibility, which
is the focus of this blog, is that of automimicry, in which some fraction of
the individuals in a defended species produces little or nothing of the
defence, but nevertheless obtains some protection from predation because they
are identical in appearance to their defended conspecifics (Speed et al. 2006).
In automimicry, the replication of patterns present in
conspecific individuals at homologous sites or elsewhere in the body can take a
few forms. It can occur through behaviour where an individual will mimic the
gestures of another class of individuals, and it can also occur through
morphology where colour patterns and threatening body parts are repeated and used
for intimidation and defence (Guthrie and Petocz, 1970). Automimicry seems to
follow the concept of one works well, and two work even better (Guthrie and
Petocz, 1970). So basically, as long as an automimic prey species looks and
acts like it has that secondary defence, even though it doesn’t, it is usually
avoided by predators.
There are many examples of automimicry. Some include the
gray hairstreak butterfly (Strymon
melinus) which have a false head at the base of their wings deflecting
attacks from the actual head. The foureye butterflyfish (Chaetodon casistratus) mimic their own eyes on their back to also
deflect attacks from the vulnerable head. Also many males of various bee and wasp species, although
defenceless, are protected from predators by their resemblance to females that
are equipped with stingers.
![]() |
The gray hairstreak butterfly (Strymon melinus) Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Automimicry#/media/File:Gray_Hairstreak_(One_more_time...)_(6222138633).jpg Retrieved on: 03/05/2019 |
![]() |
The foureye butterflyfish (Chaetodon casistratus) Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Automimicry#/media/File:Chaetodon_capistratus2.jpg Retrieved on: 03/05/2019 |
References
Guthrie, R.D. and Petocz, R.G., 1970. Weapon automimicry among mammals. The
American Naturalist, 104(940), pp.585-588.
Ruxton, G.D. and Speed, M.P., 2005. How can automimicry persist when
predators can preferentially consume undefended mimics?. Proceedings of the
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1584), pp.373-378.
Speed, M.P., Ruxton, G.D. and Broom, M., 2006. Automimicry and the
evolution of discrete prey defences. Biological Journal of the Linnean
Society, 87(3), pp.393-402.
Svennungsen, T.O. and Holen, Ø.H., 2007. The evolutionary stability of
automimicry. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1621),
pp.2055-2063.
Automimicry seems to be quite a curious thing. I assume it’s something to do with underlying genetic changes that result in the secondary defence (e.g. the toxin) not being expressed?
ReplyDelete